Stat 301 – Review 2 Problems Solutions

 

1) Weights of 30 (fun-size) Mounds candy bars and 20 (fun-size) PayDay candy bars, in grams, are shown in the dotplots below.

(a) Which distribution would you consider skewed to the right?

The Mounds distribution is a bit skewed to the right and the PayDay distribution is strongly skewed to the left.

(b) Which distribution do you expect has a larger mean?

The PayDay distribution is clearly centered around larger values than the Mounds distribution.

(c) Which distribution do you expect has a larger standard deviation?

The PayDay values are more spread out/less consistent than the Mounds distribution.

In other words, the Mounds weights are more consistent, but occasionally a few weigh more.  The PayDay distribution is less predictable and often has weights that are much lower than typical, perhaps the difference of one or two peanuts?

(d) Which distribution would you suspect will have its mean larger than its median?

Mounds because it is skewed to the right

 

2) The highway miles per gallon rating of the 1999 Volkswagen Passat was 31 mpg (Consumer Reports, 1999). The fuel efficiency that a driver obtains on an individual tank of gasoline naturally varies from tankful to tankful. Suppose the mpg calculations per tank of gas have a mean of  = 31 mpg and a standard deviation of  = 3 mpg.

(a) Would it be surprising to obtain 30.4 mpg on one tank of gas? Explain.

Not really, 30.4 is well within one standard deviation of the “population” mean of 31.

z = (30.4 – 31)/3 = -0.20

 

(b) Would it be surprising for a sample of 30 tanks of gas to produce a sample mean of 30.4 mpg or less? Explain, referring to the CLT and to a sketch that you draw of the sampling distribution.

First, does the CLT apply here?  We don’t know much about the shape of the population distribution, though it’s reasonable to assume the mileage from different tanks will by symmetric and roughly normal.  But we also don’t care too much because our sample size of 30 is considered large.  We are also assuming these observations are taken under identical conditions.

 

So we will model the distribution of the averages of 30 tanks for be normally distributed with mean equal to 31 mpg and standard deviation equal to 3/sqrt(30) = 0.5477 mpg.

 

So a sample mean of 30.4 mpg would be (30.4 – 31)/.5477 = -1.095 standard deviations below the mean.  This is still not larger than 2.

 

Using the normal distribution, P(< 30.4)

Graphical user interface, application

Description automatically generated

About 13.7% of random samples of 30 tanks will have an average mileage of 30.4 mpg by random chance alone.  We would probably not consider this a surprising outcome (happens more than 10% of the time).

 

(c) Assess the validity of your calculations in (a) and (b)

It’s always reasonable to calculate a “standard score” as I did in (a).  If I wanted to convert this z-value to a probability, then I would need to know that the tank MPGs follow a normal distribution. We aren’t told that here though it seems a reasonable assumption.  As stated in (b), we can use the CLT if we continue to have this belief in the normality of the MPG values in general or if it’s not too crazy behaving because then the sample of size 30 tells us that the distribution of sample means should still be approximately normal.

 

If you go to the Sampling from a Finite Population applet and check the box for Population Model, you can simulate drawing random samples from a probability distribution rather than a finite population. When the probability distribution is a normal distribution, everything works very well:

 

If the theoretical probability distribution is not normal but symmetric, things still work pretty well.

 

 

If the theoretical probability distribution is not normal to begin with, things still work pretty well due to the “large” sample size

 

3) The file AgeGuesses.txt contains students’ guesses of my age on the first day of class a few years ago.

(a) Estimate and interpret a 95% confidence interval for the population mean.

 

Confidence interval for the population mean:

 + t* (s /) = 48.43 + t* (10.89/sqrt(30))

 

For 95% confidence, we estimate t* to be around 2.

48.42 + 2(1.99) = (44.4, 52.4) years

I’m 95% confident that the average guess of my age in the population of all Cal Poly students on such an activity would be between 44.4 and 52.4 years

 

(More precisely, t* = 2.045)

 + t* (s /) = 48.43 + 2.045 (10.89/sqrt(30)) = (44.36, 52.50).  A little bit wider.

 

On an exam without the computer, for 95% confidence you can use 2 for either z* or t*. That’s why I said “estimate”

 

(b) Estimate and interpret a 95% confidence interval for the next student’s guess of my age.

 + t* (s ) = 48.43 + 2 (10.89 × sqrt(1+1/30)) = (26.29, 70.6)

I’m 95% confident that any one Cal Poly student would guess my age between 26.3 and 70.6 years.

 

(More precisely)

 + t* (s ) = 48.43 + 2.045 (10.89 × sqrt(1+1/30)) = (25.79, 71.07)

 

(c) Which interval do you feel is more meaningful in this context?

Opinions will vary, the prediction interval is quite wide due to the huge amount of variation in the responses given to this question.  Typically a prediction interval is more meaningful (what will happen next, vs. what is the long-run mean), but because it’s so wide this one is not very informative, basically saying I went to graduate school but I’m still alive!

 

(d) What information would you need to know to decide whether students’ are “biased” in how they guess my age in this activity?  If you did a test of significance, would this be a one-sided or a two-sided test?

You would need to know my actual age, then we could see whether the sample mean fell above that (overestimating my age on average) or below that (underestimating my age on average).

 

(e) Evaluate the validity of your calculations in (a) and (b).

The distribution is pretty symmetric and the sample size is 30 so the confidence interval in (a) is probably ok (achieves the stated 95% confidence in the long run), but with the outliers on both sides, the sample distribution of age guesses has heavier tails than we might expect for a normally distributed population. If we believe these long tails exist in the population, then this would cast some doubt as to the validity of the prediction interval (though again, at least the distribution is symmetric, but there may be less than 95% of the population distribution falling within 2 standard deviations of the mean, or more if the population standard deviation is inflated by such outliers).  The nonlinear nature of the normal probably plot suggests these data are not coming from a normally distributed population.

(f) Column 2 indicates whether the data were collected in Section 1 or Section 2.  I changed something about my appearance between the two sections. Suppose I find a statistically significant difference in the average guess of my age between the two classes, flipping a coin in advance to decide which appearance I would use in each section. Would you be willing to attribute the change in the ages to the change I made in my appearance? Explain why or why not.

While I did randomly assign the two treatments in a sense, I did so at the class level rather than at the individual student level.  So there could still be a confounding variable between the two sections (e.g., I looked more tired later in the day) and we should not draw any cause-and-effect conclusion here. (Actually the average guess was 10 years larger in section 1!)

 

4) In a recent study (Klein, Thomas, and Sutter, 2007), researchers found that current smokers were more likely to have used candy cigarettes as children than current nonsmokers were.

(a) Identify and classify the explanatory and response variables.

EV = whether used candy cigarettes as child

RV = whether or not current smoker

 

(b) When first hearing of this study, someone responded by saying, “Isn’t the smoking status of the parents a confounding variable here?”

Explain what “confounding variable” means in this context, and describe how parents’ smoking status could be confounding (i.e., describe what would need to be true).

It would be a confounding variable if it provides an alternative explanation for the observed association. To do this, it must differ between the explanatory variable groups and potentially impact the response variable.  So if those with smoking parents are more likely to be allowed to play with candy cigarettes as children but also more likely to smoke due to the environment they were raised and/or genetics, then the smoking habits of the parents might better predict who is a later smoker, but would also explain why current smokers are more likely to have played with candy cigarettes.

 

 

5) Newspaper headlines proclaimed that chocolate lovers live longer, following the publication of a study titled “Life is Sweet: Candy Consumption and Longevity” in the British Medical Journal (Lee and Paffenbarger, 1998). In 1988, researchers sent a health questionnaire to men who entered Harvard University as undergraduates between 1916 and 1950. The study included 7841 men, free of cardiovascular disease and cancer. From the questionnaire they determined whether the respondents consumed candy “almost never” (3312 men) or “sometimes or often” (4529 men), and then they tracked the participants to determine whether or not they had died by 1993.

(a) Identify the observational units.

men

(b) Identify the response variable.

Whether or not the person had died by 1993.

(c) Identify the explanatory variable.

Whether the person was classified a candy consumer (sometimes or often) or not a candy consumer (almost never)

(d) Was this an experiment or an observational study? If an experiment, was it a randomized, comparative experiment? If observational, was if a case-control study? This was an observational study because the candy-consumption levels were not imposed on the men in the study, the men in the study chose for themselves.  This is probably best classified as a cohort study because they were identified, their candy consumption determined, and then followed for 5 years to determine the outcome for the response variable. This means its legitimate for us to use this data to estimate the probability of still being alive.

(e) Researchers found that of respondents who admitted to consuming candy regularly, 267 had died by the end of 1993, compared to 247 of the non-consumers of candy. Set up the calculation for Fisher’s Exact Test for deciding whether candy consumers are significantly less likely to have died than non-consumers by completing the following:

Note: The conditional proportions of death are 267/4529 = .05895 and 247/3312 = .07458

Best bet is to set up the two-way table:

 

candy consumer

non-consumer

Total

still alive

4262

3065

7327

Died

267

247

514

Total

4529

3312

7841

 

If we let X represent the number still alive in the candy consumer group, then we want to find above X (even more survivors in candy consumer group)

 

          p-value = P(X > 4262 ) where X follows a hypergeometric distribution with parameters

          N =     7841                       M =    7327             n =     4529

 

We can also look at the number deaths in the candy consumer group, which we expect (in the long run) to be less than the number of deaths in the non-consumer group. In this case, p-value = P(X < 267) where X follows a hypergeometric distribution with parameters N = 7841, M = 514, and n = 4529.

(There are other correct set ups as well.)                   

 

(f) Suppose you wanted to carry out a simulation to determine how surprising it is for two random samples from the same population to give a difference in sample proportions at least this large.  Describe the simulation process (if describing an applet, name the applet and the input information you would use).

We would randomly sample 4529 men and 3312 men, each from a population with a probability of success of 7327/7841 = 0.934

Then we would count how many samples have a difference as extreme (one-sided alternative hypothesis) as 0.05895 – 0.07458 = -0.0156.

 

For fun:

Chart

Description automatically generated with medium confidence

With such large sample sizes, this is extremely statistically significant.

 

Also notice that even though random sampling is probably a better model here, the FET p-value is quite similar as well.

 

(g) The study reported: Between 1988 and 1993, 514 men died: 7.5% of non-consumers, but only 5.9% of consumers (age adjusted relative risk 0.83; 95% confidence interval 0.70 to 0.98). Interpret this statement as if to someone who has never taken a statistics class.  In particular, what do you think is meant by “age adjusted relative risk”?

This interval provides an assessment for how much less likely a candy consumer is to die in this time frame than a non-consumer. The values in the interval are all less than one, so if we knew the death rate of non-consumers, we would multiply by .70 to .98 to find the death rate for those who eat candy. 

“Age adjusted relative risk” essentially looks at the relative risks in different ages groups (so only comparing men of similar ages) and then roughly averages across those values to get an age-adjusted relative risk. This helps ensure we have “controlled” for age since we couldn’t do random assignment.

 

(h) Based on this interval, I would consider the comparison statistically significant.  Why?

Yes, because 1 is not inside this 95% confidence interval, we know the two-sided p-value is less than .05.

 

(i) This does not appear to be a large difference (7.5% vs. 5.9%), are you surprised that this result is statistically significant? Explain.

1. No because the relative risk takes the magnitudes of the values into account: 1.6 percentage points may not be a lot but it’s a decent fraction of 5.9%.

2. The sample sizes are pretty large so even a weak association will probably end up being “statistically significant.”

 

(j) The study also reports: We then examined different levels of candy intake. Compared with non-consumers, the relative risks of mortality among men who consumed candy 1-3 times a month (1704 men), 1-2 times a week (1589 men), and 3 or more times a week (1236 men) were 0.64 (0.48 to 0.86), 0.73 (0.55 to 0.96), and 0.84 (0.64 to 1.11),

Does this result provide evidence of a “dose-response”? Explain.

Yes, the relative “risk” of surviving that long is increasing with increasing amounts of candy!

 

(k) And then: Finally, using life table analysis truncated at age 95, we estimated that (after adjustment for age and cigarette smoking) candy consumers enjoyed, on average, 0.92 (0.04 to 1.80) added years of life, up to age 95, compared with non-consumers.

Based on these results, are you willing to conclude that eat candy leads to a longer life?

No, this was not a randomized comparative experiment, so we can’t draw any cause-and-effect conclusions.

A possible confounding variable is “happiness” – those who are happy and relaxed and not worried about what they eat are more likely to consume candy than those who are stressed and worried and watching their diet closely.  But that happier lifestyle may also be responsible for longer lives.

 

(l) What population are you willing to generalize these results to? Explain.

At most well-off males (graduates from Harvard), but even that is risky as this study did not involve random sampling. It’s possible the access to medical care and long-life span for such individuals is not representative of all adults (certainly not women).

 

6) A study of whether AZT helps to reduce transmission of AIDS from mother to baby (Connor et al., 1994): Of the 180 babies whose mothers had been randomly assigned to receive AZT, 13 babies were HIV-infected, compared to 40 of the 183 babies in the placebo group.

(a) Create a segmented bar graph to display these results. Comment on what the graph reveals.

This bar graph (and the conditional proportions of 13/180 vs. 40/183) indicates that mothers given the placebo were about 3 times as more likely to have babies that were HIV positive than were the mothers given AZT.

 

(b) Check the validity conditions for whether a two-sample z-test can be applied to these data. Be sure to mention whether the study involves random sampling from populations or random assignment to treatment groups.

The number of successes and failures in each group should be at least 5.  The four values are 13, 180-13 = 167, 40, 183-40=143. This condition is met.

(c) If you were to carry out a simulation to obtain a p-value, would you simulate random sampling or random assignment?  Explain.

The data are from randomly assigning subjects to two treatment groups.  So our p-value will want to reflect the random variation from random assignment (e.g., shuffling the 363 cards (53 successes and 310 failures) to groups of 180 and 183).

 

(d) Conduct an appropriate test of significance to determine whether the data provide convincing evidence that AZT is more effective than a placebo for reducing mother-to-infant transmission of AIDS. Report the hypotheses, test statistic, and p-value. Also indicate the test decision using .01 as the level of significance.

The null hypothesis is that AZT and a placebo are equally effective in reducing mother-to-infant transmission of AIDS.  Specifically, the probability of HIV-positive babies born to mothers who could potentially take AZT is the same as the probability of HIV-positive babies born to mothers who could potentially take a placebo. In symbols, the null hypothesis is H0: πAZT - πplacebo = 0.

      The alternative hypothesis is that AZT is more effective than a placebo for reducing mother-to-infant transmission of AIDS, or that the probability of HIV-positive babies born to mothers who could potentially take AZT is smaller than the probability of HIV-positive babies born to mothers who could potentially take a placebo.  In symbols, the alternative hypothesis is Ha: πAZT - πplacebo < 0.

Because this is a randomized experiment and the counts are on the small size, we could carry out Fisher’s Exact Test.

Or we could carry out the random assignment simulation

And find the p-value by counting how many re-random assignments have a difference in proportion with HIV positive babies (AZTplacebo) of -.146 or less

Or, because we said in (b) that the theory-based approach should be valid, we could go straight to the Theory-Based applet to carry out a ‘two-sample z-test’

With such a small p-value, reject H0 at the .01 level of significance.

We have very strong statistical evidence that AZT is more effective than a placebo for reducing mother-to-infant transmission of AIDS. We can say ‘more effective” because this was a randomized, comparative experiment.

 

(e) Estimate the relative risk of transmission with the placebo compared to AZT with a 95% confidence interval. Also be sure to interpret this interval in context.

Sample relative risk (with placebo in numerator): (40/183)/ (13/180)  = 3.03

For 95% confidence, z* = 1.96

For relative risk, we first take the ln rel risk: ln(3.03) = 1.106

SE(ln rel risk) = sqrt(1/40 – 1/183 + 1/13 – 1/180) = 0.3015

Confidence interval: 1.106 + 1.96(.3015) = (0.515, 1.697)

Back-transforming: exp(0.515, 1.697) = (1.67, 5.46)

We are 95% confident that the probability of transmission is 1.67 to 5.46 times higher with the placebo than with AZT.

(f) Summarize the conclusion that you could draw from this study (significance, estimation, causation, and generalizability). Also explain the reasoning behind each component.

 

Because this was a well-designed experiment with a small p-value, we can conclude that AZT caused the observed difference in HIV transmission rates.  If AZT and a placebo were equally effective in reducing mother-to-infant transmission of AIDS, we virtually never see sample results as or more extreme as those we saw in this experiment by random assignment alone (p-value < .0001). We are 95% confident that using the placebo increases the probability of transmission by 67% to 546%. We might have some caution in generalizing these results to a larger population as we don’t know how the HIV-positive mothers willing to participate in this study were recruited. 

 

7) Consider the question of whether exposure to second-hand smoke is harmful to the health of children. EV = whether or not exposed to second hand smoke, RV = health of child

(a)  Describe a prospective cohort observational study that could address this question.

Find children who will be exposed to second hand smoke and children who won’t.  In a few years, compare the health of the two groups.

(b)  Describe a retrospective case-control observational study that could address this question.

Find healthy children and unhealthy children and then see how much second-hand smoke they were exposed to growing up.

(c)   Describe a cross-classified observational study that could address this question.

Find older children and then determine their health status and whether they were exposed to different amount of second-hand smoke.

(d)  Describe how you could (in principle) design an experiment to address this question.

Randomly assign some children to be exposed to second-hand smoke and some children not to be exposed to second-hand smoke.

(e)  Would it be ethical to conduct an experiment to address this question?  Explain.

Because second-hand smoke is so potentially hazardous, it would not be ethical to willing impose this treatment on children.

 

8) Investigation 3.10

(a) The observational units are drivers; Explanatory variable is whether or not got a full night’s sleep in previous week; Response variable is whether or not were involved in a car crash; This is a case-control observational study.

(b)

 

No full night’s sleep

Full night’s sleep

Total

Crash

61

535-61 = 474

535

No Crash

44

588 – 44 = 544

588

Total

105

1018

1123

(c) Because this was a case-control study we should use the odds ratio. If we look at the odds of being in a crash if they did not get a full night’s sleep compared to the odds of being in a crash if they did get a full night’s sleep: (61/44)/(474/544) = 1.5911

Note, this is the same as the odds of not getting a full night’s sleep for the crash victims vs. the odds of not getting a full night’s sleep for the non-crash subjects.

The odds of being in a crash if didn’t get a full night’s sleep were 1.59 times higher than the odds of being in a crash if did get at least one full night’s sleep.

(d) We could replicate the sampling design by sampling independently from two binomial processes with the same probability of success (this will model H0: = 1). One process will represent the sampling of the crash victims (n = 535) and the other will represent the sampling of the no-crash population (n = 588).  We can use 105/1123 » 0.581 as the common probability of success.  Once we get the two samples, we will calculate the simulated odds ratio.  Then we will see how often we get a sample odds ratio of 1.5911 or larger (Ha: > 1, note, it’s not clear here whether they had a one or two sided alternative in mind, but it’s reasonable to think that they suspected the lack of sleep would be associated with an increase in odds of a car crash.).

(e) Let Xcrash be the number of successes (no full night’s sleep) in the crash group, so we are modeling Xcrash as binomial with n = 535 and  = 0.0935.

Let Xno crash­ be the number of successes in the “no crash” group, and we are modeling Xno crash as binomial with n = 588 and p = 0.0935.

Then odds ratio = (Xcrash/Xno crash) / [(535 - Xcrash)/(588 - Xno crash)]

The distribution should appear skewed to the right with mean close to 1 and standard deviation near 0.2.

Example results:

Chart, histogram

Description automatically generated

mean = 1.108, standard deviation = 0.2129

The observed ratio of 1.59 is a fair bit out in the tail of the distribution and appears to have a smallish p-value.  If we count how many of these observations are 1.59 are larger (rounding down from 1.5911 so that 1.5911 is included), we find about 1% (10 out of 1000) of the simulated sample odds ratios are at least this extreme. Using Fisher's Exact test, the p-value is 0.0158 = P(X ≥ 61, for X hypergeometric with N = 1123, M = 535, n = 105. This gives us strong evidence, of a relationship in the population.

(f) Theoretical standard error for the log odds ratio:

 »  0.2075

ln(1.59) + 1.96(0.2075) = 0.4637 + 0.4067 Þ (0.057, 0.870)

exp(0.057, 0.070) Þ (1.06, 2.39)

We are 95% confident that the odds of being in a car crash are 1.06 to 2.39 times larger for those without a full night’s sleep in the previous week compared to those with at least on full night’s sleep.

This interval does not capture one, so we have statistically significant evidence of an increase in odds for those without a full night’s sleep.

 

R output (R inverts a test (doubling the one-sided p-value) rather than using the z-formula)

Text

Description automatically generated

JMP output

Graphical user interface, text, application

Description automatically generated

These are very close to the confidence interval we calculated.

(g) We have strong evidence (p-value = 0.0158) that sleep deprived drivers have higher odds (4% to 140% higher) of being in a car crash, at least for drivers like these New Zealand drivers. We cannot draw a cause-and-effect conclusion however as this was an observational study.  We should probably apply these results only to drivers in New Zealand at that time.